Unconscious Thinking (ch. 15, Reisberg)
Chapter 15 of the textbook argues that much of our thinking goes on
unconsciously, and this fact turns out to have important implications for
the legal system. In many trials, for example, the judge knows that jurors have
been exposed to prejudicial pretrial publicity. The judge may ask jurors,
therefore, whether they think they'll be able to set aside what they've heard
prior to the trial, and decide a verdict based only on the trial evidence. But
how accurately can the jurors answer this question? Can the jurors forecast what
they'll be influenced by? Then, at the time of the verdict, will jurors know
whether they were influenced by the pretrial publicity? Research on the
cognitive unconscious suggests pessimistic answers to these questions.
Similar concerns apply to the trial's witnesses. Imagine, for example, that the
police show you six photographs and ask you to pick out the man who robbed you.
After you make your choice, the police officer smiles and says, "Yes, that's who
we suspected. By the way, how certain are you in your selection?" Then the
police ask some additional questions: How good a view did you get of the
perpetrator? How far away was the perpetrator? For how many seconds was the
perpetrator in view?
In this situation, notice that you received a bit of confirming feedback, right
after the identification ("Yes, that's who we suspected."), and this has a
powerful influence: As we mentioned in Chapter 7 of the textbook, participants
given this sort of feedback report that they are more confident in their choice,
compared with participants not given feedback. Participants given this feedback
also end up remembering that they paid closer attention, had a better view, and
were able to see more of the perpetrator's face, in comparison to people not
given this feedback. Of course, these recollections are mistaken-because, in the
experiments, the feedback and no-feedback groups got exactly the same view and
could see exactly the same details.
Why does feedback have these effects? It is as if participants were saying to
themselves, "I guess I can set my doubts to the side, because the officer told
me that I did get it right. With my doubts now suspended, I suppose I can say
that I'm certain. And since I apparently chose the right guy, I guess I must
have gotten a good view, been standing close enough, and so on."
This certainly sounds as if participants are drawing inferences from the
feedback and reaching conclusions about what they did or didn't see. But this
reasoning happens unconsciously. Participants are aware only of the product that
results-a certain level of confidence in their identification, and a set of
recollections about the event. And there is nothing exotic about the unconscious
status of these inferences. Instead, this is the way cognition works in
general-with most of our thought process hidden from view.
In these and other ways, research on the cognitive unconscious suggests that
jurors and witness do not know their own minds, and this raises serious
questions about some courtroom and police procedures. It is plausible that we
need to adjust some of our procedures and not count so heavily on witnesses' and
jurors' knowing the nature, and sources, of their own thoughts.
To learn more about this topic in cognitive psychology and the law:
See, in the textbook chapter, pages 512-517
Wells, G. L., Olson, E. A., and Charman, S. (2002). The confidence of
eyewitnesses in their identifications from lineups. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 11, 151-154
Bradfield, A. et al. (2002). The damaging effect of confirming feedback on the
relation between eyewitness certainty & identification accuracy. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 87, 112-120.